Research Article |
Corresponding author: Jean-Louis Rallu ( rallujl@gmail.com ) © 2024 Jean-Louis Rallu.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation:
Rallu JL (2024) Tahiti Population (Re)Estimates and Ideologies. Population and Economics 8(2): 231-262. https://doi.org/10.3897/popecon.8.e116822
|
The question of Tahiti population at contact was first addressed by assessing the reliability of early and later estimates, and then by a retrodiction based on the first reliable censuses and data on trends. Cook’s famous estimate of 204,000 Tahitians, affected by a wrong number of districts, has been reworked based on his own and the Forsters’ observations, using coast length and the age and sex structure of Tahiti population at contact in relation to infanticide, resulting in a range of 156,000-188,000 persons in 1774, before Boenechea’s flu in 1772 and the 1774 flu epidemics. The discrepancies between Wilson’s 16,050 and other missionaries’ estimate of 50,000 as of 1797 are puzzling, but the 1829-1830 missionary estimates were strongly affected by Tahitians’ resistance to Christianization. The twentieth century estimates are affected by a political context related to colonization - decolonisation, leading to the fact that researchers favour low estimates. McArthur uses missionary data, assuming that baptisms are births, although before the conversion was completed, baptisms included adults and children of all ages. She bases her estimate of 30,000 Tahitians at contact in 1767 on Wilson’s number of 16,050, who applied the size of nuclear households, 6 persons, to clans. Oliver followed her, however not excluding numbers twice as high or more. Our retrodiction on the 1848 and 1881 censuses, based on qualitative missionary reports on sanitary situation and data of Rarotonga that experienced similar events as Tahiti, yield, with conservative assumptions, 180,000 persons at contact, showing consistent results with early navigators’ estimates. My own PhD experience in the late 1980s shows that scholars’ re-estimates were constrained, directly or indirectly, by political considerations related to the guilt syndrome and colonialist ideology. This appears in the frequent contradictions found in their works to support low estimates in order to reduce the decline, despite evidence of dramatic epidemics and a steady decline extending almost throughout the 19th century. The same attitude of denial is found in some missionary, administrative and medical reports, particularly on the Marquesas in the late 19th century. Colonisation was associated with various ideologies, related to anthropology’s early attempts based on theories of evolutionism, functionalism and structuralism, occasionally resulting in fictions such as “primitive thinking”, “ahistorical societies” and “collective unconscious”, that were part of the justification of colonisation and its mistakes.
population estimates, missionary data, population decline, “political” pressure on research, guilt syndrome, colonialist ideologies
This second paper on post-contact decline in Tahiti population comes in the frame of the two methods to assess contact-era population size, either based on discoverers’ estimates and reports, or by retrodiction based on the late 19th century censuses with available missions and colonial administrations’ data and medical reports on the state of health (
Estimates of island populations have been a subject of controversies starting nearly from their origins, with very different numbers advanced by early navigators. The most typical case is Tahiti, with Boenechea’s 10,000 and Cook’s 204,000 estimates, and a wide range of numbers between. However, it is quite rare to have several estimates for an island at contact and at the turn of the 19th century, making Tahiti a particularly interesting case.
Most of debates are focused on how to reconcile early estimates with later missionaries’ estimates or “censuses”, more or less accurate 19th century administrative censuses and scholars’ re-estimates in the 1960s. This endeavour was often affected by guilt syndrome and politics, as shown by my own experience, in the frame of revisionist approaches and post- and neo-colonialist ideologies. It now involves several sciences: archaeology with data on remains of housing structures, superficies of garden land and carrying capacity, medicine and epidemiology, and demography.
We shall focus on statistically based estimates at contact in the frame of the reports of the first European witnesses of Tahiti traditional society, socio-political situation and practices to regulate population growth, such as infanticide, and compare them with retrodictions based on administrative censuses in the second half of the 19th century. Later estimates will be evaluated based on trends projected based on early estimates in comparison to retrodictions, using missionary and other reports and, when data are lacking for Tahiti, reliable data of the Marquesas and Rarotonga that experienced similar situations. Re-estimates by scholars in the 20th century will be considered in relation to early estimates and population density, and scrutinized for inconsistencies revealing political or other pressure. This will help us to trace ideologies that shaped colonial and post-colonial interpretations of the population decline in Pacific Islands to anthropological theories developed in the early 20th century by evolutionism, functionalism and structuralism, and the latter’s return in the 1970s at the time of independences.
In the late 18th century, discovery expeditions were aimed at gathering information on resources of newly found lands to expand colonisation. They included scientists, botanists and even painters. Most of European discoverers were surprised by a high population density in Tahiti and some other Polynesian islands and attempted to estimate, but the estimates turned out to be often “guesstimates”. However, when opportunities allowed it, some are based on statistical methods.
Polynesian societies were already in the advanced state of development, with large religious structures, marae, and double-hulled war-canoes, the largest ones carrying around 100 rowers and warriors, or even more. Tahiti consists of two peninsulas, Tahitinui and the smaller one - Taiarapu, that were divided into districts and sub-districts with various alliances between them, forming a fragmented and unstable political context (Fig.
There are several testimonies that the island was crowded. Robertson, Wallis’ second mate, reported gardens, plantations, orchards and many houses scattered on the coastal plain (
Few estimates of island populations in the 18th and 19th centuries are based on some kind of basic statistical methods, like those done by Cook and the Forsters. Robertson’s estimate of over 100,000 in 1767, is based on a circumnavigation of the island and a hike in a valley. The first data available relating to Tahiti population before 1800 is Tupaia’s list of numbers of trained warriors, taatatoa, by district, equaling to 6,780, given to Banks in 1768, that does not allow for an estimate, because the proportion of its members in the population is unknown. In 1774, Cook and the Forsters were invited to a review of a fleet preparing an attack against Eimeo (Moorea) - according to Cook, the Forsters,
The reliability of the missionary count in 1829-1830 will be assessed with information on progress of Christianisation while the first administrative census in 1848 will be corrected based on our retrodiction – a method also called backwards projection – based on the 1881 census (section 2). Sporadic missionary data on the late 1820s cannot be used to assess population trends and civil registration data in 1852, with missing years, are incomplete, mostly in terms of deaths, but they can be corrected.
Re-estimates in the 20th century will be assessed for their consistency with Robertson, Cook and the Forsters’ reports and our retrodiction.
Wallis stayed in Tahiti, in Matavai, for five weeks from June 19, 1767. He circumnavigated the island and his second mate, Robertson, reported seeing houses all along the coast and thought it was “the most populous country I ever saw” (
At his second voyage, on April 26th, 1774, Cook attended the review of the fleet of two districts «Attahuru and Ahopatea» (
Later, Cook was told that the expedition was to include 210 war-canoes, with 4 districts combining forces, the two districts of Atehuru, Faaa and Matavai (
However, Cook’s famous estimate is not based on the April 26, 1774 review, but on the review, of Faaa’s fleet that took place a few days later; the fleet consisted of 40 war-canoes, yielding, with 40 men per war-canoe and 43 districts (24 in Tahitinui and 19 in Taiarapu, but some of the names he considered to be districts, were actually sub-districts), 68,000 men (actually 68,800) and, with adult men representing one third of the population, a total of 204,000 inhabitants (
We should also reconsider the ratio of total population to adult men: 3, used by Cook, because it may not apply to Tahiti at contact in the frame of the particular population trends since the arrival of Tahitians about 1200 years before present, leading to infanticide to reduce population growth in the two centuries prior to contact (
It is possible to do another estimate based on coast length, a method used by Tahitians and Europeans as well. To reduce variations in districts size, we use the four districts preparing the expedition against Eimeo. These districts had 240 war-canoes
A review of the fleet is an excellent opportunity to estimate population, because it is much easier to count people in a few canoes to get an average number of men in them and extrapolate the results to the number of canoes by type, than to estimate de visu a crowd on an open space. Thus, using coast length and information on districts participating in the review, provides estimates that are not far from the number that Cook wrote in his log. Based on Faaa alone, our method yields 142,000 and 177,000 inhabitants, respectively, with 40 and 50 men on war-canoes. Cook’s estimate is wrong because the number of districts is inaccurate, 43 instead of 21, and second, his ratio of the total population to adult men - 3, is about 6 per cent too high.
Assuming about the same decline - 15 per cent for the flu brought by Boenechea in 1772, the first flu virus affecting Tahitians, and the 1918 flu - 14.7 per cent (
Compared with the above results, Boenechea’s estimate of 10,000 in 1772 is totally unbased. As his crew brought a flu that Tahitians reported to Cook to have caused many deaths, they probably kept away from the Spanish. In a strong contrast, Rodriguez, after a tour around the island in 1774, noted that it was «very populous […] pretty densely peopled», etc. Hence, the Vice-Roy of Peru raised Boenechea’s number by 50 per cent, resulting in a mere 15,000 inhabitants that is sometimes attributed to Rodriguez (
Bligh comments on cured sexually transmitted diseases and mostly, a large excess of births over deaths (
As soon as missionaries arrived in Tahiti in 1797, they observed that the population was much less numerous than reported by Cook and, after a tour around the island, they estimated it at about 50,000 (
We evaluate the 1829-1830 missionary “census” in relation with our retrodiction of the 1881 administrative census. The missionaries’ estimate of 8,674 Tahitians in 1830 was carried out from July 1829 to June 1830, a too long span to be accurate, and it is not clear whether all the population was counted. Davies and Darling «listed the numbers of church members, baptized adults and children and unbaptized adults and children» (
After considering the completeness of Tupaia’s list of warriors,
McArthur mostly bases her estimates on Wilson’s number of 16,050 in 1797, discarding that ti and matteina are clans, rather than nuclear households of six persons on average. She estimates population at contact in 1767, using a model resulting in a decline by 50 per cent in 30 years, based on a stable population with life expectancy at birth of 30 years for males and females, increasing by 1 per cent yearly (in the absence of infanticide) and a rate of infanticide of 75 per cent – that we assume to be non-sex selective because she does not state it (
Missions probably have exaggerated infanticide. Williams estimated its frequency on accounts of only three women reporting having killed 21 newborns altogether, without stating how many were allowed to live ( |
Oliver analyzes Cook’s review of the fleet on April 26, 1774, and, similar to McArthur, he attributes it to the whole island, discarding Cook and the Forsters’ reports and a fragmented socio-political situation presented in both Arii Taimai and Teuira Henry’s books that he could not ignore. Moreover, he considers that taatatoa represent all adult men which is surprising for a specialist in Tahitian history. After rounding Tupaia’s total to 7,000, he estimates the total Tahiti population at 35,000, with a factor 5. However, in the last section of his book, he writes that the population of Tahiti in 1797 was «by conservative estimate ... about a quarter of its previous size» (
There is little (much less than in Cook and the Forsters’) methodology in McArthur, Oliver and Beaglehole’s estimates, except for the former’s model of decline associated with a hypothetical estimate of the level of infanticide that is hardly documented. All reject Cook and the Forsters’ observations and estimates, assuming that all the forces of Tahiti participated in the April 26 review, dismissing their reports that there were only 2 districts, Faaa being reviewed on May 14 (
McArthur and Oliver’s estimates of 30,000 and 35,000 people represent between 157 and 183 persons, or 26 and 30 households, per kilometer of coastline. What would be the need of practicing infanticide of 75 per cent of births with such a low population density? Mostly, it is inconsistent with Robertson and also Rodriguez – observations of a dense population, that the former estimated at over 100,000. How could these authors consider that the witnesses of Tahiti at contact, Robertson, Cook, Forster, etc., the latter two having statistical skills, circling the island and walking through the valleys during their stays of altogether six months or more, were mistaken to the point of confusing such sparse villages with a dense population of 100 or 150 persons or more per sq.km?
It is still more surprising that such widely acknowledged specialists in Tahiti history and demography as Beaglehole, Oliver and McArthur contradict themselves so frequently: Beaglehole hesitantly balancing between high and low estimates; McArthur mentioning several severe epidemics yet making her calculations excluding epidemics; Oliver contradicting his own estimates. This shows that they were all subject to pressures to produce low estimates of Tahiti population at contact. Note that before the takeover by France in 1842, Tahiti was mostly ‘administered’ by missionaries of the London Missionary Society (LMS), receiving supplies from Australia. Therefore, they couldn’t but consider Tupaia’s list of taatatoa as representing all adult men and the fleet review including all Tahiti forces.
McArthur carried out a detailed study of demographic trends in Pacific islands, based on the mission data, but she also quoted missionaries’ reports around 1829, claiming that they did not know the number for those «not united with them» (
Missions recorded baptisms and deaths of the converts. In the early days of Christianisation, baptisms mostly included adults who eventually baptized their children as well, therefore, it is impossible to know the number of infants/newborns who have been baptized. We can estimate infants at 14 per cent of the baptisms in Rarotonga, based on returns from missionary Pitman in 1827-1833 ( |
McArthur and Oliver’s ’official estimates of 30,000 and 35,000, respectively, are also flawed for a simple reason, they imply that annual rates in 1767-1848: -1.4 per cent and -1.6 per cent
When I started my PhD in 1985, I was planning to correct Cook’s 204,000 estimate for the erroneous number of districts, 21 instead of 43, yielding 100,000, and raise it to 110,000 to be in the average with Forster’s 120,000 estimate. I was strongly suggested by my demographer colleagues, who knew only of the two numbers, Cook’s 200,000 and McArthur’s 30,000, to stay on the conservative estimate of Tahiti population. They stated that the former was «pure fantasy» or «totally irrational» while the latter could be «reasonably revised» by 20 per cent or so, which is an important revision in contemporary demography. I answered them that it was useless to do a PhD and write a book on this issue, and I insisted that it was necessary to, at least, double McArthur’s number to be in agreement with recent archaeological findings.
Following these suggestions/constrains, I have extrapolated Cook’s estimates of men, 7,760, participating in the fleet review on April 26, 1774, based on Tupiai’s numbers of taatatoa by district, despite the weaknesses of his list (see above). To remain within the acceptable range I was given, it was necessary to discard Cook’s report that two districts participated in the review, therefore I included seven districts, from Faaa to Vaiari
Talks with Patrick Kirch and his team in Maui in 2002 showed that much higher numbers than 70,000 or 80,000 are possible, and they advocated for sticking to Cook’s estimates. But I was still reluctant to accept a density of 150 p. per sq.km or more. Thus, returning to my PhD project, I suggested to finalize a draft retrodiction of population based on the corrected 1848 and 1881 censuses, yielding 110,000 at contact (
The second reconstitution scenario (
Later mentions of epidemics are often very imprecise. According to
I assume that a decline during the flu epidemics in 1772 and 1774 was 17 per cent and 13 per cent, respectively, on average similar to the 1918 flu in Tahiti (
I assume a steady decline in non-epidemic years after the 1772 flu, reaching 2 per cent by 1785, and increasing to 3 per cent by 1795, as contacts have intensified since 1788, mostly due to tuberculosis with “Swellings under their ears”
I assume that the decline remained high in the 1830s, at an average rate of 3 per cent in the aftermath of the 1820s crisis, like in Rarotonga (
Under the above assumptions, Tahiti population was somewhat above 180,000 at contact in 1767; around 100,000 in 1790-1791 when Morrison did his estimates, and about 68,000 in 1797 – his colleagues’ estimate was much closer to the real situation, and between 16,000 and 15,000 in 1829-1830. The overall decline by 1881, with 5,960 Tahitians (
While, based on the European historical demography, most demographers consider that epidemics were followed by recovery in Tahiti as well as anywhere else (see below), reliable data of many Eastern Polynesian islands suggest a constant decline throughout most of the 19th century. The Tahiti population growth recorded by civil registration data in 1852-1863 and quasi stability until the late 19th century are just artifacts of incomplete data. In 1852-1863, birth rates were just slightly above 30 p. 1,000, which is consistent with STDs, and death rates were between 20 p. 1,000 and 25 p. 1,000 (
The population trends in Tahiti are similar to those in other Eastern Polynesian islands. Reliable Rarotongan data from the late 1830s, enable us to assess a long-term decline with the balance of births and deaths remaining negative until at least 1860 and most probably the 1880s (McArthur 1964: 172-174; see also
Based on Cook and Forsters’ reports on the 26 April fleet review and probable impacts of the flu epidemics in 1772 and 1774, Tahiti population in 1767 was between 170,000 and 200,000 or somewhat above, suggesting that, with 180,000 inhabitants at contact, our second retrodiction is within the acceptable range and still conservative, because the input rates of declines due to epidemics and endemic diseases are below those recorded in other Eastern Polynesian islands where data are reliable. Thus, both approaches produce consistent results and Tahiti population at contact was most probably around 200,000, with a density of 190 p. per sq.km, which is considered acceptable by many archaeologists, because houses of commoners, without stone platforms, cannot be identified.
Given possible coercion, it is useless trying to guess various reasons, ideological, political or else, why a researcher – on her/his own or under pressure – favours lower or higher population estimates at contact.
The passionate debates on the island population estimates at contact reveal a wide range of attitudes from guilt, denial, and acknowledgment to justification, related with moral, political and ideological attitudes, embracing many disciplines: medicine, political science, anthropology, etc.
The first navigators understood that they had broken into a pure state of mankind and brought new diseases that were deadly for Polynesians. This raised the question of guilt and the dilemma of acknowledging or denying, which still appears in physicians’ reports on the Marquesas Islands in the late 19th century, in the frame of the well-established justification practices by colonial administrations. In the early 20th century, several anthropologists, mostly in Melanesia, suggested various, sometimes fancy, interpretations of the reasons for decline, avoiding to mention the lack of health services available to native populations. In the second half of the 20th century, demographers, anthropologists, historians and other scholars entered the scene with a strong denial of the huge population decline, slashing early navigators’ estimates to minimize Europeans’ responsibility in the context of the political shock of independence. Early anthropological theories, evolutionism, functionalism and structuralism fueled the ideologies inferiorizing native population, with “primitive thinking”, based on structuralism’s inaccurate kinship systems, ignoring the complexity of Pacific islands’ societies, considered as ahistorical by functionalism (
Responsibility and self-esteem, or the guilt complex, have an important role in attitudes towards collapses of native populations after their discovery. It started with a feeling of culpability for breaking into a “pure state of mankind”, and soon resulted in accusing others, as was the case with the introduction of syphilis: Wallis, Bougainville or Cook?
As regards epidemics, the discovery expeditions wrongly considered that the duration of travel would prevent the spread of viruses, discarding American experience that resulted in a quarantine being occasionally enforced in New York and Boston from the first half of the 18th century (
Ignorance of an incubation period made it possible to deny the introduction of diseases throughout most of the 19th century (
The Marquesas, with extremely high mortality until 1923, reveal interesting attitudes as regards responsibility for a population decline. With little reliability of early estimates, Du Petit Thouars’ estimate at takeover in 1842 - 20,200 is usually used as a reference. However, Jouan’s rough estimate of 11,900 in 1856 was often preferred by colonial administration because it reduces the decline under the French rule.
Dr. Lesson should be praised for a rare acknowledgement of a terrible meningitis epidemic – or rather a kind of genocide – in a valley of Nuku Hiva in 1844, linking it to the recent arrival of French colonists who gave Marquesans adulterated alcohol (
A similar attitude towards the claim that syphilis was not the cause of low birth rates because yaws confers immunity against it was common. However, yaws does not affect fertility, except maybe in its late phase, and not all suffered from yaws, therefore high rates of spontaneous abortions and still-births show that many people were affected by syphilis. Gonorrhea, a cause of sterility, was also present, but it is rarely mentioned because women ignore that they are infected and it is a cause of sterility, making it little visible (
In 1881, Dr.
Then he states that Marquesans are affected by rather banal diseases, «except leprosy that, alone, kills more indigenous people than all other diseases together» (
It is clear that he avoids to incriminate colonists, nearly supporting sales of adulterated alcohol, and colonial administration for the quasi absence of health services. It is a complete denial, ending with a strange diagnosis of «psychological disease» that Rivers was the first anthropologist to make into a theory in the 1920s.
In the 1970s, many demographers in Europe believed that plague, smallpox, and dysentery were the only catastrophic epidemics; while measles, flu, whooping cough, etc., were just minor epidemics. Most of them, except for those who worked in rural Africa in the 1960s, Caldwell, Canterelle – who have warned me that a population decline in the Marquesas and Tahiti population at contact was not a good PhD topic, etc., ignored the dramatic impact of epidemics on populations lacking immunity after centuries of isolation. Based on the European historical demography, they argued that epidemics mostly anticipate deaths and are followed by recovery. But it does not always happen. Deaths among the elderly rejuvenate age-pyramids, but this beneficial effect fades rapidly away, and epidemics affecting young children reduce birth rates when they enter reproductive ages. However, in the case of low immunity, people of all ages are more or less affected and there is little or even no rejuvenation of the age structure. In Pacific islands, epidemics were too frequent to allow for recovery and, due to low fertility and increasing mortality from endemic diseases there was no recovery at all (
Thus, many demographers I talked to about my PhD project have rejected a high impact of diseases that are common in Europe. They have also rejected a long steady decline that is a major component of depopulation in Pacific islands (
There are obvious political reasons, behind the denial of steep demographic declines by Beaglehole, McArthur and Oliver in the 1960s. First, the fear that newly independent governments request compensation – like African countries did for slavery – for recruitment and a long-term unavailability of health services for native people resulting in population decline and reduced size of their work force.
The colonial goal, or justification, was to civilize and modernize; a failure to achieve it in areas such as health and social equity was difficult to acknowledge. In the social, economic and political fields, colonisation was marred with disorders, discrimination, injustice, exploitation of resources and labour force. Colonies had to be profitable and they limited expenditures on health and social services for native populations to a minimum, if any, for instance in the South-west group of the Marquesas Islands.
A quick look at health services shows a lot about French administration’s attitude towards Polynesians. A military hospital was built in Papeete in 1845, while an outpatient facility for Polynesians was opened in the late 1850s. It became a place of debauche and abuse, and was closed in 1865. Despite numerous claims since 1868, including by Governor Petit, concerned with the rise in sexually transmitted diseases among troops and civil population, it took more than a decade to reopen it. In the Marquesas, there was only one navy medical doctor, based in Nuku Hiva, without a boat to visit other islands. In the South-Eastern group, a physician was appointed in Atuona in 1898, but the position was closed in 1901 due to lack of funds, and it was not restored until 1923 (
It would have been easy to reduce the Marquesan population decline. Vaccination against smallpox was carried out in Tahiti in the 1840s, but not in the Marquesas where vaccine was sent and arrived too late in 1863, resulting in death rates above 400 p. 1,000 in Nuku Hiva and Ua pou. Dr Rollin estimated that infected wounds and tuberculosis, with cervical tuberculous lymphadenitis (purulent infected ganglions) accounted for four fifths of all deaths in 1923 (
We shall now focus on the following two demographic interpretations of the population decline that are pseudo-justifications of the demographic impact of colonisation: pre-contact decline and psychological shock as a cause of post-contact decline.
Some colonial administrators and scholars stated that Pacific island populations had been already declining before contact, without any scientific evidence. It is only in the early 1990s that a number of dated archaeological sites showed a growth profile of Hawai’i population, the Dye-Komori curve, that
In Essays on the Depopulation of Melanesia,
J.R. Baker, who conducted a census of Eastern Santo in 1927, rejects psychological causes associated with the socio-cultural change, because the situation was the same in heathen bush villages, where tradition was still well alive, and in missionary coastal villages. He noted the important role of abortion, a lady «indicating all the children who were sitting round about her...[said] Close up all piccaninny here ol e die finish» (
The interpretation of the psychological cause of the decline was sometimes recast as an inability of native people to adapt to modernity, if not being supplanted by a superior race. There were also reports of native beliefs that increases in mortality were due to sorcery, eventually involving missions, which some Europeans interpreted as a proof that native people were irrational and believed in dark powers. However, Rivers and his collaborators’ work was, at that time, one of the few addressing population decline without attempting at quantifying it. The impact of huge declines in native populations, associated with unbalanced sex ratios hindering marriages, and the functioning of kinship systems, is totally missing in structuralist kinship studies.
Colonialism refers to “an ideology advocating the exploitation of territories that are not part of a political power and are considered as under-developed (economically, technologically and also culturally) […] justifying colonisation on differentialist arguments that Frederick Cooper calls «politics of difference» (Cooper 2010: 36) [...] resulting in hierarchies between people based on loosely defined criteria: language, ethnicity, race, religion […]” (
Thus, justifications of colonisation’s negative impact on Pacific island populations can be traced to evolutionism, functionalism and structuralism that consider primitive societies as ahistorical and native people following immutable and stereotyped behaviours that are more or less unconscious. It was mostly the result of inaccurate kinship systems developed by structuralism, according to which native people, following collective unconscious models, marry nearly exclusively matrilineal cross-cousins, as it appears in ethnocentric diagrams – with classificatory kinship terms translated into Western non-classificatory terminology –that in no way represent the Pacific kinship systems. Structuralism totally ignores the strategic role of matrimonial alliances in the frame of “networks”, including power, ceremonial and “religious” functions, and class status that make native peoples’ behaviours similar to Europeans’ (Rallu 2023). These pseudo-theories have inferiorised the colonized people and were used by colonialist propaganda to picture native societies as small groups of people, withdrawn and constantly at war, and Europeans were taught that colonisation was “civilizing” native people who ignored reading, writing and counting, the major themes of the justification of colonialisation. But it is false for the latter, because social life is based on matrimonial, economic, ceremonial and other exchanges that involve some skills in numeracy; bride price, funeral contributions, etc.
It is noteworthy that structuralism returned on the academic and political scene in the 1970s, mostly in France, at a time when it was trying to keep confetti of its former empire and as migration from former colonies was on the rise. It was a post- or neo-colonialist reaction to recent independence, and became a justification of discrimination, that followed colonisation.
Thus, there was a large political, social, etc. consensus, including among scientists, to support the existence of intellectual differences between Europeans and former colonised people while they were becoming politically equal, despite a low educational level of the majority of their population – due to a lack of schools. In Pacific islands, education was left to missions who mostly taught reading the Bible, a proof of the failure of the colonisation goal to “civilize” – or rather modernize – that was impossible to acknowledge then. Similarly, throughout most of the 19th century and sometimes until the early 20th century, colonial governments largely neglected health of the colonized populations (
Thus, the former colonial states denied, or at least minimized the tremendous declines that had affected the colonized populations. It seemed unnecessary, or a useless debate, to estimate the extent of past declines, when fertility was increasing in the developing countries and declining in the West, offering a good opportunity to obliterate the question. Although African populations were growing then, there were still pockets of temporary declines in some rural areas of Africa (Caldwell, Canterelle). The major change resulting from independence was a feeling that «White supremacy» was in question. In France, it had repercussions on politics until the 1980s and beyond, with a disastrous immigration policy in a social context prone to discrimination, and harsh responses of the Chirac government to requests for emancipation by its last colonies with the «Events» in New Caledonia and a ransacking of Papeete center by Tahitians on strike, not to mention foreign policies related to newly independent states.
The debates on population decline have mostly focused on Tahiti due to wide controversies on the size of its population at contact. However, it was too quickly stated that early navigators, some of them mastering statistical skills, were mistaken by crowds at the arrival of ships, forgetting their meticulous observations over several months. Based on the reports of reliable witnesses of Tahitian society, we can estimate its population at a range of 170,000 to 200,000 in 1774 and, as a lower bound, probably 200,000 at contact, if not slightly higher.
Pacific Islands provide well documented cases of dramatic epidemics and a long-term decline due to endemic diseases, with extreme crises over several years, halving the population in a decade or so. A retrodiction on the 1881 and 1848 censuses, based on rather conservative rates comparative of those recorded in other Eastern Polynesian islands, yields 180,000 persons at contact, showing that the above estimates of early navigators are rather plausible. These two different approaches show that in 1881 Tahiti population was reduced to about 3 per cent of its size at contact. Archaeological and carrying capacity data now show that Pacific islands populations at contact were much closer to early navigators’ than to scholars’ estimates in the 1960s.
It was difficult to acknowledge such a disastrous outcome of an enterprise that was supposed to civilize other people, and colonisation was related with various ideologies. In the frame of unprecise medical knowledge in the 19th century, doctors were at the forefront of interpreting, acknowledging, justifying or denying population collapses. Early anthropological theories have been affected by ethnocentric misunderstandings resulting in fictions, such as «primitive thinking» and its various forms, including by renown scholars, and used as justifications of colonialism. Thus, there was a large consensus to minimize the responsibility of colonisators for numerous failures.
There are reports on a rapid decline in native populations in the Caribbean islands and mainland Americas, with population being reduced by 95 per cent or more (
Bulletin Officiel de Tahiti (1848) Tableau du Recensement des Iles Tahiti et Moorea, fait le 1er février 1848.
Le Messager de Tahiti, 23 févier 1882. Recensement de la population des îles Tahiti et Moorea.-Année 1881, p. 64.
District | Warriors | District | Warriors | ||
(a) | Present name | (a) | Present name | ||
Oteihouroo | Atehuru | 400 | Whapiano | Hapaino | 200 |
Parapara | Papara | 900 | Whidia | Hitia | 600 |
Wyuridde | Vaiuriri (Papeuriri) | 300 | Whahite (b) | Afaahiti | 400 |
Opora | Pare-Arue | 800 | Tiarreboo (b) | Taiarapu | 1800 |
O’whao | Mahaena | 200 | Wyourou (b) | Vairao | 200 |
Tettahu | Faaa | 200 | Matawii (b) | Mataoae | 400 |
Matavii | Matavai (Mahina) | 200 | Wyaru | Vaiari | 180 |
Author of estimates | Dates in Tahiti | Duration of stay | Estimates | Method | Circumstances/Comments |
Robertson | 19.06-27.07.1767 | ~5 weeks | >100,000 | Observation | Circumnavigated and visited a valley |
Bougainville | April 1768 | 9 days | No | Same description of the island as Robertson | |
Cook | 13.04.1769 | 3 months | No | Tupaia’s number of warriors taatatoa cast - 6,780 provided to Banks | |
Boenechea | 20.11.1772 | 1 month | >10,000 | unbased | Brought the flu |
Boenechea | 11.1774 | A few weeks | No | Brought the flu | |
Rodriguez | 1774-1775 | 1 year | ~15,000 | Corrected by the king of Peru | Dense population; missionaries rejected/confined |
Boenechea | 11.1775 | < 2 weeks | No | Repatriate missionaries | |
Cook | April-May 1774 | 204,000 | Faaa’s fleet | Count of war-canoes, warriors, rowers; adult men = 1/3 total population (see text) | |
Forster G. | April-May 1774 | >120,000(1) | 26.04.1774 fleet review | id. | |
Forster J. R. | April-May 1774 | >120,000(2) | 26.04.1774 fleet review | id. separately for Tahitinui and Taiarapu | |
Forster J. R. | April-May 1774 | 170,660-204,800 | carrying capacity | Estimates based on bread fruit trees | |
Morrison | 1789-1791 | 19 months | ~30,000 | unbased | |
Missionary Wilson | 1797 | 16,050 | List of 2,675 clans | Done soon after his arrival, assumes a clan is a nuclear household of 6 persons | |
Other LMS missionaries estimate | 1797 | 50,000 | unbased | Travel around Tahiti |
The question of the age structure of Tahitian population at contact is important because it directly affects the estimates based on Cook and the Forsters’ observations in 1774. They used a factor 3 to estimate the total population based on their counts of adult men in the fleet reviews of 2 districts and later Faaa, on which Cook based his estimate, after extrapolating to all of Tahiti.
Archaeological data show that the population of Hawai’i stabilized in the 16th century, about 200 years before contact, and oral tradition reports that infanticide was introduced from Tahiti, that certainly experienced a similar population development. The absence of most diseases, including childhood diseases, common in Europe and other continents in Polynesia resulted in lower infant and children mortality and higher life expectancy at birth than recorded in historical demography of Europe, for 30 years. Life expectancy before contact might have reached 40 years or somewhat above, resulting in a rapid growth since the discovery of the Eastern Pacific islands by Polynesians between 1000 and 800 years before contact according to archaeological data.
The level of infanticide is not well-assessed, and it was probably exaggerated by missionaries, based on a few women of Tahitian aristocracy, without mentioning if it was sex-selective. Bligh was told that 3 children were left alive, but it is not clear what Tahitians meant by this. As other people, they were well-aware that mortality reduces the number of children, making it necessary to have more children to achieve an ideal family size, that would be 3 children, to avoid population decline and loss of power in a context of rivalry to reign over the whole islands or archipelagos. There were also probably different behaviours as regards fertility and infanticide between aristocracy and commoners.
We estimate infanticide based on a TFR of 6.9 without infanticide – a rate recorded in the Marquesas islands in 1936-1945 – and “3 children”, resulting in a rate of 57 per cent. But it is an indirect and biased estimate, because the decision to kill, or not to kill a newborn was probably related with the survival of previous children, and some children we assume to have been killed, actually replaced natural deaths.
Note that this exercise differs from MacArthur’s model because it seeks to estimate the structure of Tahiti population at the time of contact, whereas MacArthur aims to assess the decline from contact in 1767 to Wilson’s estimate in 1797, assuming a population structure at the time of contact based on a 30-year life expectancy and an annual growth rate of 1 per cent, and dramatically introducing an infanticide rate of 75 per cent. We now know from
In model A, we estimate the structure of Tahitian population at contact based on life expectancy at birth of 30 years, and 40 years in other models, using West Regional Model Life Tables (Coale and Demeny 1983); TFR is 3 in all models. The age pattern of fertility used in our models is that of the Marquesas in 1936-1945, in the frame of very permissive sexual behaviour from a young age making missionaries desperate to eliminate sexual promiscuity, with high rates in 15-19, peaking at 20-24 years (
With life expectancy at birth of 30 years and a rate of infanticide of 57 per cent for both sexes
With life expectancy at birth of 40 years and the same infanticide rate as in model A, the population still declines, but only at a rate of 0.4 per cent or by 18 per cent in 50 years. The overall sex ratio is 95. Most probably, life expectation was a little higher than 40 years or fertility was somewhat higher than 3, to allow for a stable population, in the political context.
With life expectancy at birth of 40 years, and a moderate sex-selective infanticide of 52 per cent for boys and 61 per cent for girls, and a sex-ratio at birth of 128
With life expectancy at birth of 40 years, and sex-selective infanticide rates of 49 per cent for boys and 64 per cent for girls and a sex ratio at birth of 150, the population declines rapidly, at a rate of 1 per cent yearly, or by 39 per cent in 50 years. The sex ratio of the population at the end of our simulation is 143.
It would be surprising that a high sex-selective infanticide was practiced in Tahiti where the social status of women was rather high, with women owning land and accessing chiefdom. Nevertheless, as a minor sex-selective infanticide rate of females strongly increases the sex ratio, its consequences are important for marriages. However, the permissiveness of Eastern Polynesian sexual life made it possible to reduce its consequences on population trends. In the Marquesas, polyandry was attested for some women, mostly chief women. In Tahiti, some women were district-chief or “queen”, as Europeans called them, but without any mention of polyandry. However, adulterine bastards are reported in chiefly families, sometimes resulting in arguments. In the frame of very unbalanced sex ratios, a family size of 3 children could be, not per mother, but per father, i.e. patrilineal families. Permissive sexual behaviour and unstable unions easily allowed to have on average 3 children per male head of the family, with an average number of children per women to achieve this goal according to the sex ratio of the population of reproductive age, which could be reached with less infanticide. There was no need of frequent polyandry, strictly speaking, to achieve this. However, practices were different in aristocratic families, as regards both fertility and infanticide.
Our 4 models just replicate stable populations associated with model life tables. Yet no population has experienced a stable fertility and mortality over 50 years. Following several centuries of rapid growth, Eastern Polynesian populations progressively resorted to infanticide to reduce growth and stabilize population in the 200 years preceding contact. Moreover, Pacific islands were affected by famines and tsunami, frequently reported in the Marquesas and affecting also other islands. There were cases, however rarely, of slaughter or forced emigration following wars, but people also emigrated in search of new islands to settle. Besides the need to protect their resources and political influence, the Polynesian islands populations used various ways to survive on tiny lands.
Let us return to the question of the age structure of Tahiti population at contact.
Cook and the Forsters counted men on a few war- and transport-canoes and multiplied the average numbers by the number of canoes by type. G. Forster said that there were men on the shore, but he did not include them, and he probably did not attempt at counting them, because people on the shore were constantly moving. We can assume that, if taatatoa warriors were adults in the prime of life, rowers were of varied ages; thus, all the men in canoes could be in the age group of 15-59.
The ratios of total population to men aged15-59 are 3.14 and 3.12 in models A and B, respectively; they are little different because the age structure is mostly related to fertility (the same in all models) and the age pattern of mortality is little different in life expectancies of 30 and 40 years.
This ratio is 2.81
Population estimates with the ratio of total population to men aged 15-59 years in model B adding up to 3.12, are 173,400 and 209,200, respectively with 40 and 50 men per war-canoes, which is 4 per cent higher than with Cook’s ratio of 3: 166,700 and 201,100; and model C, with a ratio of 2.81, yields 156,200 and 188,400 persons, respectively, which is a 6.3 per cent lower than with Cook’s ratio.
We think model C is acceptable, however is rather conservative as infanticide is based on an indirect estimate and includes children allowed to live to replace elders (Fig.
The age pyramid of Tahiti population at contact (model C). A Model C (life expectancy = 40; TFR = 3; infanticide – 52% of boys and 61% of girls; sex ratio at birth = 128). B Model C(a) (the same input that in the model C + 50% replacement rate for children died before age 10)
If only boys are replaced (model C(b), the sex ratio at birth is 150. The population declines by 0.5 per cent, or 22 per cent over 50 years, the proportions by age are 25, 63 and 12 per cent for age groups 0-4, 15-59 and 60 years and over, respectively; the overall sex ratio is 144 and the ratio of total population to men aged 15-59 is 2.63, because there are much less girls and women in this scenario. This scenario results in a rather high decline, which is not sustainable over a century or more, as Tahiti has been seemingly experiencing the same trends as Hawai’i for about 2 centuries before contact. Moreover, a rapid decline of Tahiti population over the one or two pre-contact centuries is unlikely both for ‘political’ and demographic reasons, as it would imply much higher density than estimated at contact, which is highly improbable. There were reports of many more men than women in Tahiti by visitors in the late 18th century and until the mid-19th century. The same was noticed in the second half of the 19th and early 20th centuries in Melanesia where there was no infanticide. It was due to the excess female mortality because women cannot get isolated as they take care of sick people. The same cause has increased the effect of infanticide in Tahiti, but its use was most probably much reduced in the first- or second-decade following contact, given the rapid decline due to the introduced diseases. This scenario, compared to the previous one, shows the impact of high sex-selective infanticide on population trends and structure, because the number of births is much lower due to infanticide of girls that are not replaced in case of deaths at young ages.
While the old age structure and the rather rapid population decline in model C were dubious as regards sociopolitical and cultural Tahitian practices, and implied a very high density one or two centuries before contact, its first revision with replaced dead children, model C(a), is more consistent with Tahitian historical and sociocultural features and demographic trends derived from Hawaiian archaeological data. It shows a more likely and, on the long term, sustainable pre-contact demographic situation of Tahiti, with nearly stable population size, moderately imbalanced sex ratio and younger age structure. Note that the same result could be reached with slightly different life expectation, fertility and infanticide rates.
When the Dolphin sailed along Tahiti coast, Robertson
The number of war-canoes in the second attack is consistent with Arii Taimai’s report that Hapape, the district of Matavai, had 44 war-canoes, and its western neighbor, Arue, with about the same coast length, could have about the same number of war-canoes. Thus, the first attack involved only Hapape, with the assistance of part of the forces of its western neighbor in the second attack. However, Robertson does not give a number of men on war-canoes because, given the tense situation, he had no time to count them. But the carpenters sent to destroy the war-canoes abandoned on the shore after Tahitians fled provided more precise information, they were «forty to fifty foot long and capable of carrying upwards of thirty men» (
The English had little, or even avoided, contacts with what they called or believed to be the ‘principle people’, trading mostly with an old man whom later Cook called Owhaw (
In a walk along the Matavai River, Robertson noted that there were no other trees than breadfruit, vi-apple and plantain trees, assuming that «they permit no barren trees to cumber the low grounds» (
Thus, it is clear that all the population of Tahiti did not assemble at the arrival of the Dolphin, because it did not stop and continued sailing outside the reef to find a safe pass to search for a mooring inside the lagoon. It is also clear that the fragmented political situation and the competition between district chiefs did not allow for a united attack on the Dolphin, and the same applies to the fleet reviews later seen by Cook and the Forsters.
Jean-Louis Rallu - independent researcher, retired senior researcher at INED (Institut National d’Etudes Demographiques), Paris, 93300, France. E-mail: rallujl@gmail.com